Receptiveness more important than routine for infant sleep
By ANIWednesday, August 11, 2010
WASHINGTON - Being emotionally receptive can reduce sleep disruptions and help infants and toddlers sleep better, found researchers at Penn State.
“Bed time can be a very emotional time. It heralds the longest separation of the day for most infants. It struck me that going to sleep, and sleeping well, is much easier for some young children than others, and I wanted to assess what factored into this, and what parents and children contribute to sleep patterns,” said Douglas Teti, professor of human development and family studies.
In the study, which examined mothers’ behaviors during infants’ bedtimes, parents had the most success with their children’s sleep when they responded appropriately to their children’s cues.
These include showing disinterest in an activity or simply glancing inquisitively at a parent.
For example, one mother in the study talked quietly and gently to her 6-month-old infant while breastfeeding.
“She continuously gazed at the infant’s face and, whenever the infant vocalized, she responded promptly (e.g., ‘It’s OK.’),” said the authors.
In contrast, a different mother in the study “used stern directives with her 24-month-old during book-reading whenever the child got up out of bed,” and “continually attempted to engage the child in the book despite clear signs that the child was losing interest (e.g., child was fidgety and continually turned his attention elsewhere),” note the authors.
The result: “the child got up and left the room four times before he eventually fell asleep.”
When parents provide reassurance through emotional communication, the researchers believe that it lets children know they are in a safe environment.
“Emotions are the most basic form of communication between babies and parents,” said Teti.
His findings pose new challenges to parents because they suggest that being emotionally available-paying attention to cues and responding to children appropriately-is more effective than a specific bedtime behavior in promoting better sleep.
The researchers found no significant relation between sleep disruptions and the amount of time parents spent in close contact with infants or involved in quiet activities before bedtime. This contradicts past research, which had suggested that prolonged close physical contact with a parent undermines babies’ ability to sleep on their own.
This study was one of the first to use direct observation of infant sleep patterns, and is the first to use multiple video cameras in the infants’ and parents’ bedrooms to capture parent-infant interactions at night.
“Sleep is a context about which we know little. It can be a very emotionally charged period for parents and babies. Looking at parent-child interactions in this context could be more telling for childhood outcomes than what you see in a more structured daytime play session,” said Teti.
The study has been published in a recent issue of the Journal of Family Psychology. (ANI)